Is defined as a persons feeling of oneness with the emotional state of another person?

Empathy can be defined as a complex capacity requiring to share and understand other individuals’ cognitive or affective perspectives and to propose a verbal or behavioral answer adapted to the emotions, feelings, or thoughts they expressed (Decety & Jackson, 2004;

From: The Handbook of Alcohol Use, 2021

Client-Centered, Bio-occupational Framework for Orthotic Intervention

Terri M. Skirven OTR/L, CHT, in Rehabilitation of the Hand and Upper Extremity, 2021

Orthotic Pearl: Exercise in Empathy

When students are learning orthotic skills, a useful exercise is to have them wear a hand orthosis fabricated by a fellow student for a 24-hour period. They will experience first-hand (pun intended) how having one or more joints immobilized can affect function. Also apparent are discomfort from pressure points, the attention that the orthosis attracts in public, and the heat and perspiration retained by the orthotic material. This experience reinforces the importance of:

Optimal joint positioning

No unnecessary restriction of joint motion or function

Optimal appearance, comfort, and convenience

Discreet design

Environmental considerations when selecting materials and design

Considering cultural and spiritual factors

The Psychological Correlates of General Factor of Personality

Janek Musek, in The General Factor of Personality, 2017

Empathy, Altruism, and Prosocial Behavior

Empathy is the next psychological construct that has an important role in social relations and can be also related to altruism. Both constructs can be classified into the domain of prosocial behavior denoting acting for the benefit of the others. Empathy, altruism, and prosocial behavior are crucial in interpersonal relations and have been often associated with evolutionary and genetic factors as well as personality (Figueredo, Woodley of Menie, & Jake Jacobs, 2016; Musek, 2010; Rushton, Bons, & Hur, 2008; Rushton & Irwing, 2011). Therefore, we can assume that they are also connected to the GFP. Darwin (1871) already emphasized prosocial and cooperative behavior as characteristic of human beings and altruism with empathy represents a distinctive feature of the K reproductive strategy demanding prolonged care for the offspring (Rushton, 1985).

Prosocial behavior is clearly related to personality dimension including the Big Five (for a review of the literature, see Habashi, Graziano, & Hoover, 2016; Mlčák, 2012). However, direct connections of the GFP to prosocial behavior were only scarcely investigated although we can make assumptions on the Big Five links to the prosocial traits. Besides, the relationship between empathy and personality is complex because some components of empathy include seizing the distress of other persons. Thus, a different direction of the relation is expected for nondistressing components of empathy as compared to distress-taking components. For example, neuroticism should correlate positively with distressing components of empathy and negatively with nondistressing components.

Indeed, the GFP might only modestly correlate with the empathy in general. In the MIDUS II American representative sample (N = 4963; Ryff et al., 2007), we can calculate the scores for the Big Five (measured by Midlife Development Inventory/MIDI/Personality Scales; Lachman & Weaver, 1997; Rossi, 2001), extract the scores for the GFP and correlate them with the scores of Sympathy Scale (Uchida & Kitayama, 2001), which is closed to the empathy by definition. Pearson correlation coefficient between the GFP and sympathy is significant but rather small (.19). Agreeableness is most related to empathy among the Big Five dimensions (r = .37). Empathy is also included in some personality inventories. Rushton and Irwing (2009) analyzed the data obtained on Comrey Personality Scales and reported substantial saturation of the empathy factor on the GFP (.36).

O’Connor et al. (2015) compared 2409 meditation practitioners to 450 nonmeditators on different psychological measures including the personality dimensions (Big Five and GFP, extracted from BFI: John et al., 1991), empathy (perspective taking, empathic concern, and personal distress, measured by Interpersonal Reactivity Index: Davis, 1983) and altruism (measured by Compassionate Altruism Scale; Berry, O’Connor, Rangan, & Stiver, 2012). The practitioners of contemplative meditation had higher GFP, agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness, emotional and cognitive empathy, and altruism, yet lower depression, neuroticism, and empathic distress. The GFP thus positively correlated with the components of empathy and altruism that do not involve distress and negatively with the distress-taking components.

Altruism is a construct close to empathy and it is certainly included into the personality, especially into the agreeableness dimension. Rushton et al. (2008) confirmed the link between the GFP and measures of altruism. The GFP accounted for 56% of the reliable variance in measures of altruism, personality, and temperament. In practically all analyses of higher-order structure of the Big Five, agreeableness (A) was considerably saturated on the GFP (see Fig. 1.4 and Table 3.3).

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128112090000054

The Interviewer's Questions

Mark H. Swartz MD, FACP, in Textbook of Physical Diagnosis: History and Examination, 2021

Empathy

Empathy is a response that recognizes the patient's feeling and does not criticize it. It is understanding,not an emotional state of sympathy. You try to put yourself in the shoes of the patient. The empathic response is saying, “I hear what you're saying.” The use of empathy can strengthen the doctor–patient relationship and allow the interview to flow smoothly. Studies have shown that high levels of empathy have been linked to better patient outcomes!2

Examples of empathy are the following:

“I'm sure your daughter's problem has given you much anxiety.”

“The death of someone so close to you is hard to take.”

“I guess this has been kind of a silent fear all your life.”

“You must have been very sad.”

“I know it's not easy for you. I'm delighted to see that you're trying to eat everything on your tray.”

“That's wonderful that you have stopped smoking.”

The last two examples illustrate an important point: it is critical to give credit to patients to encouragetheir role in their own improvement.

It is impossible, however, to put yourself in the shoes of the patient because of a difference in age, gender, life experiences, education, culture, religion, and other factors. An extremely empathic statement, which sounds counterintuitive, is “It's impossible for me to fully understand how you are feeling, but how can I help you? How can we work together to get past this difficult problem?”

Empathetic responses can also be nonverbal. An understanding nod is an empathetic response. In certain circumstances, placing a hand on the shoulder of an upset patient communicates support. The interviewer conveys that he or she understands and appreciates how the patient feels without actually showing any emotion.

Empathetic Technology

Néna Roa Seïler, Paul Craig, in Emotions, Technology, and Design, 2016

Introduction

To start looking at empathy and technology, we begin by looking at the philosophical and psychological underpinnings of empathy. Next we consider the relationship between empathy and technology, and look at some existing models of empathy related to technology. We continue by considering the concept of the virtual agent as an empathetic companion and describe some examples of virtual companions in commercial and academic settings. To conclude, we look at the results of user studies focused on understanding our perception of virtual companions and describe what is required to develop an appropriate companion interaction strategy.

The chapter will give the reader grounding in the essential theory of empathy and its relationship with technology. It will also introduce state-of-the-art empathetic agents and describe how experimental methods such as the use of the Kelly grid and Semantic Differential can be used to discover how users feel about emergent technologies. The chapter will be of interest to application designers or developers working with interfaces that employ agents or have agent-like behavior, or anyone who wants to find out more about the developing relationship between empathy and technology.

What is Empathy?

Empathy is the ability to understand and respond to the unique affective experiences of another person (Decety & Jackson, 2006). If we consider empathy at the level of experience, it is essentially a psychological construct that denotes a sense of similarity between one's own feelings and those expressed by another person. Empathy can be also seen as an interaction between two individuals who share each other’s experiences and feelings, although this exchange of feelings does not necessarily mean that one will act or even feel compelled to act in support or sympathize. Indeed, the social and emotional situations that arise due to empathy can be quite complex. These depend on the feelings experienced by the observed person, the relation of the target to the observer, and the context in which the social interaction occurs.

Since the age of classical antiquity, empathy has been seen as important because it allows us to be able to understand the thoughts of others and to predict or explain what others feel, think, or do. It has also been seen as having a central role in ethical thinking and behavior. The “Golden Rule” of ethical reciprocity (Wattles, 1997) relies fundamentally on the psychological process of empathy. This is often cited as a pillar of the Christian faith (Armstrong, 1994), classically stated in the Bible as “Do unto others as you would have them do unto you,” and is prominent in other religious traditions such as Hinduism, Buddhism, Taoism, and Zoroastrianism. Empathy is also considered to have a central role in the esthetic understanding of our engagement with works of art and fictional characters. Following on from these philosophical and religious underpinnings, a more complete understanding of empathy is now offered by the interaction of research in science and humanities (Coplan & Goldie, 2011).

Recently, empathy has received much attention from philosophers, psychologists, and cognitive neuroscientists. Here, studies have documented that empathy plays a central role in moral reasoning and pro-social behavior that motivates and inhibits aggression toward others. Batson, Duncan, Ackerman, Buckley, and Birch (1981) are part of this movement, offering an empathy-altruism hypothesis which states that reliable, purely altruistic action can only happen if it is preceded by empathic concern for another. Other authors define empathic concern as an emotional reaction characterized by feelings such as compassion, tenderness, sympathy, and soft heartedness (Decety & Lamm, 2006). Conversely, a lack of empathy is seen as leading to aggressive, antisocial behavior (Miller & Eisenberg, 1988) and cruelty (Baron-Cohen, 2011). Other authors consider empathy as socially oriented emotion, defined as “the ability to put oneself into the mental shoes of another” (Goldman, 1993), a complex form of psychological inference (Ickes, 1997), an affective response more appropriate to someone else’s situation (Hoffman, 1982), another-oriented emotional response (Batson et al., 1997), or an affective response that stems from the apprehension or comprehension of another’s emotional state (Eisenberg, 2000). In the field of cognitive neuroscience, there is a further distinction between cognitive empathy which is the ability to know what another person is thinking and feeling, and affective empathy which is the ability to actually feel another person’s emotional state (Rueckert & Naybar, 2008).

Bringing these different definitions together, empathy is essentially the ability to imagine or feel ourselves in the position of others, and it is generally expected that this insight will guide us toward more ethical or moral behavior. Empathy allows us to perceive the experience and feelings of others as if those experiences and feelings were shared, and encourages us to improve the situation of others as if it were our own. These feelings of empathy can also help explain altruistic and unselfish behavior where actions are exclusively for the benefit of others. Empathy also allows us to be more tolerant and accepting of others. This is particularly important in this modern age, as working and learning environments become increasingly diverse due to high levels of global migration (Hollingsworth, Didelot, & Smith, 2003; Joplin & Daus, 1997). Indeed, common empathy is often cited as an important factor for cohesion and maintenance of social structures within a healthy society (Bartal, Decety, & Mason, 2011; Byrne et al., 2008; Pierce, 2008).

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128018729000041

Emotion, Empathy, and Ethical Thinking in Fable III

Karen Schrier, in Emotions, Technology, and Digital Games, 2016

Empathy-related thought processes were identified as those skills and thought processes related to emotions and feelings, the taking of other’s perspectives, and prioritization of other’s feelings or perspectives, as based on the literature review (see Tables 3.1–3.3, 3.5, and 3.6). Reasoning-related skills and thought processes were identified as those skills and thought processes related to assessing choices, thinking through consequences, prioritizing goals, or weighing pros and cons, regardless of whether it involved emotions, empathy, or neither (see Tables 3.3–3.6). A negative relationship between the use of empathy and reasoning skills and thought processes to make ethical decisions was not suggested by this study. Rather, empathy and other types of skills were used in tandem to make decisions. The use of emotions and affects in ethical decision-making, which was not at the expense of reasoning, is echoed by researchers, such as Robinson (2004) and Hanoch (2002), who reunify the rational with the emotional, and see any separation as superficial. fMRI (functional MRI) studies suggest that there are distinct areas of the brain—ones related to emotion and ones related to cognition—that are activated differentially by various types of ethical scenarios, further complicating these findings (Borg, Hynes, van Horn, Grafton, & Sinnott-Armstrong, 2006).

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128017388000038

MaPS-Teen Treatment Protocol

Mary Nord Cook, in Transforming Teen Behavior, 2015

Empathy for Relationship Building

The capacity for experiencing and expressing empathy may be the most important skill in relationships. Brooks and Goldstein (2001) cite parental modeling and teaching of empathy as a key ingredient in fostering strong parent–child relationships and bolstering resiliency, the capacity to rebound from stress. Regular expression of empathy can go a long way toward building and maintaining strong relationships and buffering children from stress. Many adults struggle to master this skill, and most parents do not actively demonstrate empathy to their youngsters. To be successful socially, however, youth must regularly employ empathy.

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128033579000052

The Intersection Between Technology, Mind-Wandering, and Empathy

Mariana Bockarova, in Emotions, Technology, and Behaviors, 2016

Toronto Empathy Questionnaire (TEQ)

The TEQ is a 16-item self-report measure used to assess trait empathy, which, in this instrument, is represented as an emotional process. As such, items in the TEQ, developed by Spreng, McKinnon, Mar, and Levine (2009), target emotional states, sympathetic physiological arousal, conspecific altruism, and empathic responding. Sample items include “Other people's misfortunes do not disturb me a great deal” (emotional states); “It upsets me to see someone being treated disrespectfully” (sympathetic physiological arousal); “I enjoy making other people feel better” (altruism); and “I get a strong urge to help when I see someone who is upset” (empathic responding). Using a five-point Likert scale, possible responses include 1 = “I never agree”; 2 = “I rarely agree”; 3 = “I sometimes agree”; 4 = “I often agree”; and 5 = “I always agree.” Although there is no benchmark defining an empathic individual, high scores generally account for high empathy. The scale demonstrated high internal consistency (α = 0.82).

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128018736000030

Personality and Behavioral Factors

Lee Ellis, ... Anthony W. Hoskin, in Handbook of Crime Correlates (Second Edition), 2019

5.1.7 Empathy

Empathy refers to the tendency to understand or to actually feel the experiences of others. It is not easily measured in precise terms, although several questionnaire scales have been developed and used (R Hogan 1969; Chlopan et al. 1985; Jolliffe & Farrington 2007).

Several researchers who work in this area have made a distinction between cognitive empathy (i.e., the knowing or recognition aspect of empathy) and affective empathy (i.e., the actual emotional feeling aspects of empathy, sometimes called moral emotions). In addition to empathy, a few of the studies to be reviewed here pertain to altruism, the tendency to have great concern for the welfare of others. These particular studies are specifically identified as such.

Sufficient numbers of studies on empathy have been reported that Table 5.1.7 is presented in two parts, one for official measures of offending and the other for all other measures. Table 5.1.7a shows that most of the findings indicate that there is an inverse correlation between empathy and official crime and delinquency.

Table 5.1.7a. Empathy and Official Offending Behavior.

Nature of RelationshipOfficial Data
Violent, Sex, or PropertyGeneralDelinquencyRecidivism
Positive
Not significant NORTH AMERICA United States: Goldstein & Higgins-D’Alessandro 2001 (affective empathy, offenders vs. non-offenders, adolescents); Lindsey et al. 2001 (sex offenders, adolescent ♂s) OVERVIEW Meta-Analysis: Jolliffe & Farrington 2004∗ (controlling for IQ & parental SES); van Langer et al. 2014 (includes both adults & juveniles, affective d = −.19) NORTH AMERICA United States: Kendall et al. 1977 (offenders vs. non-offenders); Hudak et al. 1980:Table 1 (offenders vs. non-offenders)
Negative EUROPE Britain: Farr et al. 2004 (sex offending)
NORTH AMERICA Canada: Rice et al. 1994 (rapists vs. non-rapists, ♂s); Chaplin et al. 1995 (cognitive empathy, child molesters vs. non-offenders, ♂s); D Walsh et al. 2004 (arson, ♂s)
OCEANIA New Zealand: DM Burke 2001 (sex offenders vs. non-offenders, ♂s)
AFRICA Egypt: Megreya 2015:86
EUROPE Denmark: Lauterbach & Hosser 2007 (♂s, r = .16)
NORTH AMERICA United States: Turner 1948 (altruism); R Hogan 1969 (cognitive empathy, inmates vs. military officers); Riley 1986; Mulloy et al. 1991 (♂s, r = .14); Priest, Kordinak & Wynkoop 1991 (♂s); R Krueger et al. 2007:Table 3 (criminals vs. non-criminals)
OVERVIEW Meta-Analysis: Jolliffe & Farrington 2004 (cognitive d = −.48, affective d = −.11, especially for violent, lowest for sex offenses); van Langer et al. 2014 (includes both adults & juveniles, cognitive d = −.43)
EUROPE Britain: Eysenck & McGurk 1980; Netherlands: Van der Geest et al. 2007 (♂s); Lodewijks et al. 2008 (♂s, r = .31); Schalkwijk et al. 2014 (adolescents, offenders vs. non-offenders); Spain: Llorca-Mestre et al. 2017
NORTH AMERICA Canada: R Robinson, Roberts et al. 2007; United States: Kurtines & Hogan 1972 (delinquents vs. college students); Mehrabian & Epstein 1972; Rotenberg 1974; Aleksic 1976; PL Ellis 1982 (inmates vs. non-delinquents, especially violent inmates); Kaplan & Arbuthnot 1985; WR Smith & Monastersky 1986 (♂s); M Lee & Prentice 1988 (♂s); Chandler & Moran 1990
OCEANIA Australia: Mak 1991 (emotional empathy, offenders vs. controls, ♀s)
EUROPE Spain: Romero-Martínez et al. 2016
NORTH AMERICA United States: van der Put et al. 2012:309 (adolescents)
OVERVIEW Meta-Analysis: Van Vugt et al. 2011:1243 (moral emotions—very similar to empathy, r = −.11); Vachon et al. 2014, physical aggression r = −.12; sexual aggression r = −.09)

Table 5.1.7b summarizes the evidence concerning the relationship between empathy and unofficial measures of offending. One can see that the vast majority of studies indicate that offenders are less prone to be empathetic than persons in general.

Table 5.1.7b. Empathy and Unofficial Offending Behavior.

Nature of RelationshipSelf-Reported DataClinical/Personality Indicators of Antisocial Behavior
OverallIllegal Drugs
Positive
Not significant EUROPE Britain: Jolliffe & Farrington 2007∗ (♀s, overall offending, cognitive empathy)
OCEANIA Australia: Moriarty et al. 2001:749 (sex offenses, adolescents)
EUROPE Britain: Jolliffe & Farrington 2006∗ (bullying, cognitive empathy); Jolliffe & Farrington 2011∗ (bullying, ♀s); Greece: Kokkinos & Kipritsi 2012∗ (affective empathy); Netherlands: Zonneveld et al. 2017∗ (cognitive empathy, externalizing behavior); Norway: Endresen & Olweus 2001∗ (aggression & bullying, ♀s)
NORTH AMERICA United States: MM Johnson et al. 2014:385∗ (psychopathy, cognitive empathy); TJ Dollar 2016∗ (bullying, ♀s)
OVERVIEW Meta-Analysis: Miller & Eisenberg 1988:331 (various forms of aggression)
Negative ASIA China: Ma & Leung 1991 (altruism)
EUROPE Britain: Jolliffe & Farrington 2006∗ (overall empathy); Jolliffe & Farrington 2007∗ (♂s, especially violent offending, cognitive empathy); Germany: Krettenauer & Eichler 2006 (moral emotions); Spain: Luengo et al. 1994a (both cognitive & emotional)
MIDDLE EAST Turkey: Ashraf et al. 2014 (affective empathy, r = −.28)
NORTH AMERICA United States: R Hogan 1969; Hogan & Jones 1983:17; M Schaffer et al. 2009:594 (correlation with emotional stronger than cognitive); Posick et al. 2014:Table 2
OVERVIEW Meta-Analysis: van Langer et al. 2014 (cognitive d = −.43; affective d = −.19)
EUROPE Spain: Luengo et al. 1994a:32∗
NORTH AMERICA United States: Schreiber 1992 (sales)
ASIA China: Cheng, Hung & Decety 2012:626 (when watching pain being imposed on others, psychopathy); X Wang, Lei et al. 2016:Table 1 (among juvenile offenders)
EUROPE Britain: Tranah et al. 1998:743 (CD); Warden & Mackinnon 2003 (bullying); Jolliffe & Farrington 2006∗ (bullying affective empathy); Jolliffe & Farrington 2011∗ (bullying, ♂s); ND Thomson et al. 2016 (psychopathic traits, ♀s); Finland: Kaukiainen et al. 1999 (aggression); Greece: Kokkinos & Kipritsi 2012∗ (cognitive empathy); Italy: G Gini et al. 2007 (bullying); Netherlands: de Wied et al. 2012 (disruptive behavior disorder + callous-unemotionality, adolescent ♂s); Zonneveld et al. 2017∗ (affective empathy, externalizing behavior); Norway: Endresen & Olweus 2001∗ (aggression and bullying, ♂s); Endresen & Olweus 2002 (bullying); Spain: Romero-Martínez et al. 2016 (antisocial personality); Zych et al. 2015a (bullying); Sweden: Soderstrom 2003 (psychopathy); Switzerland: Malti et al. 2009 (CD)
LATIN/CARIBBEAN AMERICA El Salvador: Olate et al. 2012:Table 3 (gang membership)
NORTH AMERICA Canada: Cohen & Strayer 1996 (CD); United States: Feshbach 1975 (CD); Heilbrun 1982 (psychopathy); MacQuiddy et al. 1987 (CD); Baumrind 1991 (physical aggression); Zahn-Waxler et al. 1995 (CD); D Cohen & Strayer 1996 (CD); Krueger et al. 1996:125 (CD); Fowles & Kochanska 2000 (CU, among preschoolers); Sandoval et al. 2000 (psychopathy); AB Loper et al. 2001 (psychopathy); Pardini, Lochman & Frick 2003 (CU); Mullins-Nelson et al. 2006 (cognitive empathy, psychopathy); Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous & Warden 2008 (adolescent psychopathy); AP Jones et al. 2010 (adolescent psychopathy, ♂s); AA Marsh et al. 2013 (adolescent psychopathy); MM Johnson et al. 2014:385∗ (psychopathy, affective empathy); TJ Dollar 2016∗ (bullying, ♂s)
OCEANIA Australia: Zágon & Jackson 1994 (psychopathy traits)
OVERVIEW Literature Review: Lovett & Sheffield 2007 (antisocial behavior); Meta-Analysis: Miller & Eisenberg 1988 (antisocial behavior, r = −.18); Rigby 1996 (bullying); Malti & Krettenauer 2013 (moral emotions & antisocial behavior); Meta-Analysis: Vachon et al. 2014 (r = −.11); Zych et al. 2015b (bullying, both cognitive & affective empathy); Zych et al. 2018 (cyber-bullying)

One meta-analysis indicated that cognitive empathy is associated with offending more than affective empathy, although both forms become non-significant after statistically controlling for intelligence and parental social status (Jolliffe & Farrington, 2004). A couple of recent empirical studies, however, have concluded that affective empathy was better than cognitive empathy at differentiating self-reported offenders from non-offenders (Jolliffe & Farrington 2007; M Schaffer et al. 2009:595).

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128044179000053

Social cognition in severe alcohol use disorder

Fabien D’Hondt, ... Pierre Maurage, in The Handbook of Alcohol Use, 2021

Empathy

Empathy can be defined as a complex capacity requiring to share and understand other individuals’ cognitive or affective perspectives and to propose a verbal or behavioral answer adapted to the emotions, feelings, or thoughts they expressed (Decety & Jackson, 2004; Green et al., 2015). As such, empathy relies on both ToM and experience sharing abilities, which elicit the activation of brain regions involved in the behavior of the person we are looking at, and include motor resonance (i.e. the activation of the motor system emerging from the simple observation of another person performing an action) and affect sharing (i.e. the activation of the affective brain system emerging from the observation of another person expressing an emotional state) (Green et al., 2015). The work by Martinotti, Nicola, Di Tedeschi, Cundari, and Janiri (2009) used the Empathy Quotient (EQ), a self-reported questionnaire, and revealed that patients with SAUD had a significantly lower level of empathy than healthy controls. Recent evidence suggests that these empathy deficits could be due to decreased cortical thickness in cerebral regions that have been linked to empathy (notably frontal regions, insula, and precuneus) (Schmidt et al., 2017). The heterogeneity in the ways empathy is conceived probably limits our understanding of deficits in patients (e.g., Thoma, Friedmann, & Suchan, 2013; Thoma, Winter, et al., 2013). However, a separate assessment of affective and cognitive empathy, which was performed by Maurage, Grynberg, Noël, Joassin, Philippot, et al. (2011) by analyzing subscales’ scores of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) and the EQ questionnaires, revealed that patients were specifically impaired for affective empathy, and thus that they had difficulties to detect and feel others’ emotional states but remained able to efficiently share and understand others’ non-emotional mental states (e.g. intentions; thoughts). Interestingly, this impairment was not associated with anxiety or depression levels but was significantly correlated with alexithymia and difficulties in interpersonal relationships. More recently, Grynberg, Maurage, and Nandrino (2017) used the Condensed and Revised Multifaceted Empathy Test (Edele, Dziobek, & Keller, 2013) in which participants are told to select one label among four to describe the affective state associated with the presented picture (40 photographs of people, 20 in a positive affective state and 20 in a negative one) and to rate on a Likert-type scale how much they are likely to share the affective state of the person in the picture. While patients did not differ from controls regarding affective sharing, they were significantly impaired in affective labeling, suggesting that empathy impairments could be mostly related to difficulties in understanding affective states. Again, these results were not significantly correlated with anxiety or depression. As a whole, in line with the results related to studies investigating ToM, it appears that difficulties in empathy among patients with SAUD mostly concern its affective subcomponent, are not related to the frequent comorbidities and may impact the quality of social interactions.

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128167205000049

Empathy and Prosocial Behavior

Amrisha Vaish, in Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development (Second Edition), 2020

Empathy and Prosocial Behavior

Empathy is an affective response that stems from the apprehension or comprehension of another's emotional state and is similar to what the other person is feeling or would be expected to feel. Empathy thus allows one to feel what the other is feeling. Sympathy is related to but distinct from empathy: It is an affective response that also stems from the apprehension or comprehension of another's emotional state but does not match the other's state, consisting instead of feelings of sorrow or concern for the other (Eisenberg et al., 1991; Jordan et al., 2016). Empathy and especially sympathy lead to prosocial behaviors such as helping and away from antisocial behaviors such as aggression. Thus, to better understand how humans experience and share others' emotions as well as the motivations behind human prosocial behavior, it is important to understand the development of these emotional responses.

The origins of empathic responding lie in emotional contagion, or the tendency to automatically mimic others' emotional expressions facially, vocally, and behaviorally, and to oneself experience traces of the same emotions (Hatfield et al., 1994). However, most researchers agree that true empathy additionally involves self-other differentiation, or the recognition that one is distinct from others and that others' inner states may differ from one's own. This ability to differentiate self from others allows one to focus on others' emotional experiences and to identify the source of one's empathic response as the other rather than oneself. Such differentiation is critical to maintain the ‘other-oriented’ nature of empathy. The cognitive ability to take others' perspectives and imagine how others feel also contributes to empathic processes by providing information about the other's situation and emotions. These emotional and cognitive processes together create a powerful and flexible mechanism for apprehending another's emotional state and, when necessary, acting prosocially to relieve the other's negative state (Hoffman, 2000).

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128093245218166

Is defined as a person's feelings of oneness with the emotional state of another person?

Empathy. is a person's feeling of oneness with the emotional state of another. bystander effect. The tendency for an individual who observes an emergency to help less when other people are present than when the observer is alone.

What is the main social emotion in altruism?

Empathy: People are more likely to engage in altruistic behavior when they feel empathy for the person in distress, a suggestion known as the empathy-altruism hypothesis. 4 Children also tend to become more altruistic as their sense of empathy develops.

What involves trying to change someone's attitudes and or behavior?

Learning Objectives Persuasion is the process of changing our attitude toward something based on some kind of communication. Much of the persuasion we experience comes from outside forces. How do people convince others to change their attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors (Figure 1)?

Is the study of how people's thoughts feelings and behaviors?

Social p sychology is the scientific study of how people's thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are influenced by the actual, imagined, or implied presence of others (Allport 1998).