Narcolepsy is associated with a relative absence of a hypothalamic neural center that produces

Articles, Neurobiology of Disease

, Anna Elisabetta Vaudano, Fabio Pizza, Andrea Ruggieri, Stefano Vandi, Alberto Teggi, Christian Franceschini, Francesca Benuzzi, Paolo Frigio Nichelli and Giuseppe Plazzi

Journal of Neuroscience 19 August 2015, 35 (33) 11583-11594; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0840-15.2015

Abstract

The brain suprapontine mechanisms associated with human cataplexy have not been clarified. Animal data suggest that the amygdala and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex are key regions in promoting emotion-induced cataplectic attacks. Twenty-one drug-naive children/adolescent (13 males, mean age 11 years) with recent onset of narcolepsy type 1 (NT1) were studied with fMRI while viewing funny videos using a “naturalistic” paradigm. fMRI data were acquired synchronously with EEG, mylohyoid muscle activity, and the video of the patient's face. Whole-brain hemodynamic correlates of (1) a sign of fun and amusement (laughter) and of (2) cataplexy were analyzed and compared. Correlations analyses between these contrasts and disease-related variables and behavioral findings were performed.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT In this study we reported for the first time in humans the brain structures whose neural activity is specifically and consistently associated with emotion-induced cataplexy. To reach this goal drug-naive children and adolescents with recent onset narcolepsy type 1 were investigated. In narcolepsy caused by hypocretin/orexin deficiency, cataplexy is associated with a marked increase in neural activity in the amygdala, the nucleus accumbens, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, which represent suprapontine centers that physiologically process emotions and reward. These findings confirm recent data obtained in the hypocretin knock-out mice and suggest that the absence of hypothalamic hypocretin control on mesolimbic reward centers is crucial in determining cataplexy induced by emotions.

Emotion-induced laughter occurred in 16 patients, and of these 10 showed cataplexy for a total of 77 events (mean duration = 4.4 s). Cataplexy was marked by brief losses of mylohyoid muscle tone and by the observation of episodes of facial hypotonia, jaw drop, and ptosis. During laughter (without cataplexy) an increased hemodynamic response occurred in a bilateral network involving the motor/premotor cortex and anterior cingulate gyrus. During cataplexy, suprapontine BOLD signal increase was present in the amygdala, frontal operculum–anterior insular cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and the nucleus accumbens; BOLD signal increases were also observed at locus ceruleus and in anteromedial pons. The comparison of cataplexy versus laugh episodes revealed the involvement of a corticolimbic network that processes reward and emotion encompassing the anterior insular cortex, the nucleus accumbens, and the amygdala.

  • cataplexy
  • emotion
  • fMRI
  • humor
  • narcolepsy
  • sleep disorders

Introduction

Narcolepsy type 1 (NT1) is a rare, life-long central disorder of excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) that frequently arises in childhood or adolescence peaking at 15 years old (Dauvilliers et al., 2007), and is prevalent between 25 and 50 people per 100,000 in the adult population (Longstreth et al., 2007). The core NT1 symptoms are EDS, cataplexy, sleep paralysis, hallucinations, and fragmented nocturnal sleep (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2014). Cataplexy is pathognomonic of NT1 and is defined as sudden episodes of muscle weakness occurring during wakefulness, typically triggered by strong emotions of positive valence (most often by laughter). In addition to cataplexy, the rapid transition from wakefulness to REM sleep is the polysomnographic marker of NT1. Patients with NT1 have difficulty in executing daily activities, socializing, and maintaining personal relationships because of cataplexy and somnolence. NT1 is caused by a lack of wake-promoting hypocretin (orexin) signaling, resulting from the loss of hypocretinergic neurons in the dorsolateral hypothalamus and leading to reduced or absent CSF hypocretin-1 (Peyron et al., 2000; Thannickal et al., 2000). The cause of hypocretin-producing cell loss is still under investigation and an autoimmune mechanism is the most likely etiological hypothesis (Partinen et al., 2014).

Cataplexy can occur with either partial or global involvement of all skeletal muscles, except the diaphragm and extraocular muscles (Overeem et al., 2011). Typically, partial attacks induce slurred speech, facial weakness and twitching of the facial muscles, and jaw or head dropping, whereas global attacks can lead to generalized muscular involvement resulting in falls to the ground (Rubboli et al., 2000; Vetrugno et al., 2010).

While brainstem mechanisms regulating cataplexy in animals have been partly clarified, the suprapontine mechanisms that induce cataplexy are largely unknown. Only recently, a key role of the amygdala and ventomedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) has been shown in the induction of emotionally triggered cataplectic attacks in the hypocretin knock-out mice (Hcrt−/− mice; Burgess et al., 2013; Oishi et al., 2013; Dauvilliers et al., 2014).

In humans, the existence of a link between cataplexy and emotions was postulated long ago. Neuroimaging studies demonstrated that NT1 patients have an abnormal hemodynamic response in processing humorous or rewarding stimuli (Reiss et al., 2008; Schwartz et al., 2008; Ponz et al., 2010b). In particular, an increased amygdala activity compared with healthy controls has been observed. Moreover, abnormal behavior in the subcortical mesolimbic reward network (including nucleus accumbens, ventral tegmental area, and hypothalamus) seems present in NT1 patients during high reward expectancy (Ponz et al., 2010a).

However, few studies characterized in humans the neural correlates during typical cataplectic attacks induced by emotional stimuli. In fact, only anecdotal case reports have been described, with conflicting findings (Hong et al., 2006; Chabas et al., 2007; Reiss et al., 2008; Dauvilliers et al., 2010). This paucity of data reflects how difficult it is to evoke a cataplectic attack in a controlled setting (such as during an fMRI experiment). Moreover, the available data were often obtained in adult patients under drug treatment or during cataplectic status after antidepressant withdrawal; therefore they are not representative of genuine emotionally triggered cataplexy.

In the attempt to fill this gap, we investigated the neural networks associated with laughter-induced cataplexy using the BOLD signal changes that occurred while viewing prolonged funny videos in a “naturalistic” fMRI paradigm in drug-naive children and adolescents with NT1 close to disease onset.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Patients were proposed to participate in the fMRI study if the following inclusion criteria were satisfied: (1) drug-naive children from 8 to 16 years old with NT1, (2) a diagnosis of NT1 according to the current criteria (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2014), (3) proven CSF hypocretin-1 deficiency, (4) cataplexy onset for no more than 2 years, and (5) video documentation of cataplectic attacks during a standardized video session (Plazzi et al., 2011; Pizza et al., 2013) no longer than 6 months before the neuroimaging experimental sessions.

From January 2014 to February 2015, 21 patients (13 males, mean age 11.3 ± 2.5 years) fulfilled the inclusion criteria and agreed to participate in the fMRI study. All subjects were right-handed (Oldfield, 1971). Table 1 reports the patients' clinical data. All patients had a complete clinical and neurological examination, conventional brain MRI, and underwent a 48 h continuous polysomnographic recording followed by a standard five-nap Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT; Littner et al., 2005) and video recordings of cataplexy as previously described (Plazzi et al., 2011; Pizza et al., 2013). Before each video session, patients were interviewed to determine their humor preferences and asked to choose between various funny videos; these were selected to best stimulate each subject. Video recordings included a 5 min baseline followed by 30 min while subjects were watching videos; children were sitting and/or standing for 10–40 min, enough time for the child to feel at ease but not long enough for the child to feel sleepy. Two experts in the field narcolepsy (F.P. and G.P.) independently analyzed the video recordings and rated the presence of paroxysmal “negative” motor symptoms during emotional stimulation. HLA-DQB1*0602 typing was conducted in all patients. Cataplexy frequency was assessed and subjective sleepiness was quantified by a modified version of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (mESS; Melendres et al., 2004).

Table 1.

Clinical, polysomnographic, and laboratory data of investigated patients

The study was performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local ethical committee; consent was obtained from parents and assent obtained from patients.

Polygraphic functional imaging protocol

Stimuli.

To maximize the chance of recording cataplexy in a “hostile” environment like the MRI scanner, we adopted a patient-specific protocol by selecting the funny videos that were previously successful in the standardized video session. Videos were digitally recorded and edited to remove commercials. The resulting videos were 8 min and 30 s in length. Patients were scanned while passively viewing an entire video without any specific task requirement. We told patients they should try to keep their head fixed, but they could smile or even laugh if the comic was funny, and that they did not have to suppress emotions. Two to three videos were presented to each patient. Following the scan, each subject was asked to rate each cartoon for humor intensity (i.e., degree of funniness) on a scale of 1 to 10, with 1 and 10 as the least and most funny, respectively. Figure 1 A is a schematic representation of the fMRI protocol.

Narcolepsy is associated with a relative absence of a hypothalamic neural center that produces

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Figure 1.

Representation of the polygraphic fMRI protocol. A, Each fMRI run was organized in 30 s of baseline during which the patient was instructed to watch a white cross on a black screen followed by one funny video. Each video was 8 min and 30 s and the relative fMRI scan time was 510 s. Two to three fMRI runs were repeated in each session for every patient. Baseline/laughter/cataplexy and physiological facial movements (see Materials and Methods) were monitored throughout by a small camcorder pointing at the patient's face. B, A 60 s representative page of video-polygraphy (32 EEG channels plus 2 channels for EMG and ECG, respectively) recorded simultaneously during fMRI acquisitions, after off-line gradient artifact suppression and ballistocardiogram removal. The midline EEG electrodes (Fz, Cz, and Pz) are displayed in a bipolar montage. Surface EMG activity is recorded from the mylohyoid muscle. Note the burst of EMG activity during the laugh and the transient loss of muscle tone during cataplexy with rapid rebound after it.

Recordings.

Data acquisition was performed in a dimly lit room during the afternoon. Patients were asked to take a brief nap before the recordings to reduce sleepiness to avoid its confounding effects. Scalp EEG (1024 Hz sampling rate) was recorded by a 32-channel MRI-compatible EEG recording system (Micromed). EKG activity was recorded from electrodes positioned over the chest; surface EMG was recorded from subchin electrodes. Foam pads were used to help secure the EEG leads, minimize motion, and improve the patient's comfort. EEG recording was relevant to exclude objective signs of sleepiness/sleep onset. The patient was constantly observed and recorded with a small camcorder positioned on the head coil inside the scanner pointing to the patient's face to obtain a split-screen video-polygraphic documentation during the fMRI recording (Ruggieri et al., 2015). The simultaneously recorded video during scanning allowed patients to be checked for physiological facial movements, laughter, and the appearance of cataplexy. For the purpose of our work, chin EMG and video recordings were essential for recognizing the exact time and duration of each cataplectic event.

Functional data were acquired using the Philips Intera system at 3 T and a gradient-echo echo-planar sequence from 30 axial contiguous slices (TR = 2.000 ms, TE = 35 ms, in-plane matrix = 80 × 80, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3 mm) over two to three 8 min 30 s runs (255 volumes per run) with continuous simultaneous video-polygraphic recording. A high-resolution T1-weighted anatomic image was acquired for anatomical reference and volumetric analysis. The volume consisted of 170 sagittal slices (TR = 9.9 ms, TE = 4.6 ms, in plane matrix = 256 × 256, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm).

Video-polygraphic data analysis

After off-line correction of the gradient artifacts and filtering of the EEG signal, the EEG data were reviewed and preprocessed according to previous published methods (Avanzini et al., 2014; Vaudano et al., 2014). EMG activity from the mylohyoid muscle was filtered between 50 and 500 Hz.

Three sleep medicine experts in NT1 (G.P., F.P., and S.V.) reviewed the cataplexy video recordings obtained in the sleep laboratory, and they collegially identified the main facial features and the polygraphic picture of the cataplectic attacks for each patient. They subsequently reviewed the video-polygraphic recordings obtained during the fMRI procedure independently to identify the onsets and offsets of the events of interest for the study including (1) laughter and smiles and (2) cataplectic “negative motor” phenomena. We considered “cataplectic attacks” only the events for which the judgments of the raters were concordant. In case of disagreement, the clinical features of cataplectic attacks of that patient, as recorded in the sleep laboratory, were reviewed and compared with the events recorded in the scan. Only when a consensus was reached was the event considered a “cataplectic event” to minimize the risk of false-positive events.

fMRI data analysis

The MATLAB 7.1 and SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging, University College London) software was used for whole-brain fMRI data analysis. All functional volumes were slice-time corrected, realigned to the first volume acquired, normalized to the MNI template, and smoothed with 8 × 8 × 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. No subject was excluded from subsequent analysis for excessive head motion.

To provide the hemodynamic maps of “laughter” and “cataplexy,” the fMRI data were analyzed within the GLM framework. For each EPI session one GLM was constructed including (1) the effects of interest (laughter and/or cataplexy), represented as blocks (with duration defined by the video) convolved with canonical hemodynamic response function; (2) the confounds, represented by 24 realignment parameters (six scan realignment parameters from image preprocessing and a Volterra expansion of these; Friston et al., 1996); and (3) the video-based physiologic facial movements (i.e., swallowing; Chaudhary et al., 2012; Ruggieri et al., 2015).

To further assess the influence of head motion, and especially to test if movement affected one condition significantly more than others (i.e., laughter or cataplexy), we quantified head motion for each subject across the entire scanning session by computing vector distances (derived from the x, y, and z translational realignment corrections) between sequential functional volumes (Moran et al., 2004). Vector distances for each time point for each subject were then assigned to “laughter,” “cataplexy,” and “baseline” conditions and submitted to one-way ANOVA.

Statistical parametric maps were generated from linear contrasts between conditions in each participant (laughter > baseline; cataplexy > baseline; cataplexy > laughter). The contrast images from each subject were then submitted to a second-level group analysis using the random-effect approach (Friston et al., 1999).

The threshold for statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 corrected for false discovery rate (FDR) or p < 0.001 uncorrected if the BOLD maps did not reveal any change at the more conservative threshold. In these uncorrected maps, an extent threshold of five contiguous voxels was applied. The resulting statistical maps were displayed in MNI space.

Finally, we performed additional whole-brain second-level correlation analyses to test for a linear relation between BOLD signal changes relative to brain responses to humor and the main clinical measurements, including age at disease onset, duration of disease, cataplexy frequency, ESS score, mean sleep latency, and number of sleep-onset rapid eye movement periods (SOREMPs) at the MSLT. We also analyzed possible correlations between BOLD signal changes and the number of observed smiles/laughter events during scanning and the postscanning ratings of the videos. These analyses allowed us to assess whether the modulation of brain responses to humor might relate to individual clinical characteristics and to behavioral variables observed during the fMRI experiment.

Results

Behavior

We recorded 77 cataplectic events in 10 patients (Table 2 shows the detailed behavioral results for each patient and fMRI run). The mean duration of cataplexy was 4.4 s, ranging from 2.1 s (Patient 17) to 5.5 s (Patients 9 and 19). Cataplexy was preceded by signs of fun in every attack, such as a smile or laugh. The video recordings acquired during the fMRI procedure allowed us to recognize in each patient the typical pathological facial features that characterize childhood cataplexy: episodes of facial hypotonia, jaw drop, tongue protrusion, and ptosis (i.e., the “cataplectic facies”; Serra et al., 2008; Plazzi et al., 2011; Pizza et al., 2013).

Table 2.

Behavioral findings of NT1 patients during EEG-fMRI

In these 10 patients smiles and laughter without signs of cataplexy also were recorded (214 events). In the remaining 11 patients no cataplexy was observed during stimulation. Six of the latter patients showed laughter (28 events), while five did not show any facial display of fun and amusement.

The mean ± SD of postscanning ratings of video funniness was 7.2 ± 2 for the scanning runs with emotion-induced cataplexy and 4.8 ± 2.6 for runs without cataplexy (t = 3.56, p < 0.001). The mean number of smiles/laughter bouts was 14.7 ± 8.5 in the runs with emotion-induced cataplexy and 5.5 ± 4.6 in the runs without cataplexy (t = 3.90, p < 0.001).

Video-EEG recordings showed that EEG and behavioral signs of sleepiness and sleep never occurred during sessions. Finally, no differences in head motion were recorded among cataplexy (0.45 ± 0.29 mm), laughter (0.42 ± 0.20 mm), and baseline vision (0.40 ± 0.25 mm; F (2,22) = 0.06, p > 0.8).

Brain correlates of cataplexy

Group-level brain correlates of cataplexy were obtained by a random-effect approach across the 10 patients with emotion-related cataplectic events.

BOLD signal increases were observed (Fig. 2 A, Table 3) in a cortical and subcortical network that included the bilateral (>right) insular cortex/frontal operculum, anterior cingulate cortex, VMPFC, amygdala, posterior thalamic nuclei, and mesolimbic reward regions including the nucleus accumbens and ventral striatum. Moreover, brainstem BOLD signal increases were observed in the posterolateral pontomesencephalic region (locus ceruleus) and in the anteromedial pons. Peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) were calculated at the cluster peak (sphere = 10 mm) of right amygdala, anterior insular cortex (AIC), and VMPFC to show the time course of the hemodynamic response (Fig. 2 B). The analysis emphasizes a prominent increase in BOLD signal in these structures with a peak at 4–6 s postevent onset.

Narcolepsy is associated with a relative absence of a hypothalamic neural center that produces

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Figure 2.

fMRI results related to cataplexy. A, Group-related (random-effect analysis) cataplexy-related BOLD findings. The BOLD changes (p < 0.05 corrected for FDR) are displayed on the axial, sagittal, and coronal T1-template image. See main text and Table 3 for details of activations. No decreases in BOLD signal were detected. L, left; R, right. B, PSTHs (solid black line; black error bars represent SEM) depicting the responses in right amygdala, right insula, and VMPFC (circles). PSTH provides a finite impulse response estimate of the BOLD response locked to the onset of the cataplectic attacks as a function of peristimulus time (time bins of 2 s, peristimulus timescale of 32 s). To derive a grand average PSTH over NT1 patients, we first computed PSTHs within each subject and for each voxel of interest within each run, then averaged over sessions, and finally computed a grand average by computing over patients. Amy; amygdala; FG, fusiform gyrus; LC, locus ceruleus.

Table 3.

Regional brain responses to conditions of interest

The involvement of the amygdala, insula, and anterior cingulate/VMPFC, resulting from the random-effect approach, was supported by single-subject analyses: amygdala and insula-positive BOLD changes were detected in 8 of 10 patients (Table 4), while anterior cingulate/VMPFC BOLD increments were found in 9 of 10 cases. Brainstem BOLD-positive signal changes were observed in 6 of 10 patients (at p < 0.05 FDR). Using a less conservative threshold (p < 0.001, uncorrected) they were observed in every patient. Table 4 and Figure 3 detail the location of brainstem hemodynamic modifications at single-subject and group levels.

Table 4.

First-level analysis results of BOLD changes related to cataplexy

Brain correlates of laughter

Group-level (16 patients) brain correlates of laughter (without cataplexy) were detected over a cortical network encompassing bilaterally the motor–premotor cortex and the ACC (Fig. 4). No BOLD changes were detected over the amygdala, insular cortex, and subcortical mesolimbic reward centers (see Table 3). In a further analysis, we considered separately the 14 runs (10 patients) during which only laughter occurred and the 15 runs (10 patients) in which laughter plus cataplexy occurred (in these runs cataplexy was included in the model and treated as a confound). Both analyses disclosed the involvement of a similar circuit related to laughter covering the bilateral motor network and ACC, confirming the whole-group results. No significant differences were detected between the two conditions at p < 0.05 corrected for FDR and even at a less conservative threshold (p < 0.001, uncorrected).

Narcolepsy is associated with a relative absence of a hypothalamic neural center that produces

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Figure 4.

fMRI results related to laughter. Group-related (random-effect analysis) laughter-related BOLD findings. The BOLD changes (p < 0.05 corrected for FDR) are displayed on the axial, coronal, and sagittal T1-template image. See main text and Table 3 for details of activations. No decreases in BOLD signal were detected. L, left; R, right.

What distinguishes cataplexy from laughter?

When compared with laughter, cataplexy demonstrated a higher hemodynamic demand over a network of cortical and subcortical regions encompassing the bilateral (>right) opercular-insular cortex (global maxima), right amygdala, bilateral ventral striatum, and right nucleus accumbens (Fig. 5, Table 3). No higher BOLD signal increases were detected in the contrast laughter > cataplexy.

Narcolepsy is associated with a relative absence of a hypothalamic neural center that produces

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Figure 5.

fMRI results related to the contrast cataplexy > laughter. Group-level BOLD findings (p < 0.05 corrected for FDR) are displayed on the axial and coronal T1-template image. L, left. See main text and Table 3 for details of hemodynamic changes.

Correlation between BOLD changes and clinical behavioral variables

We did not disclose any significant linear relationship between cataplexy-related BOLD changes and clinical polygraphic variables. Similarly, we did not find any correlation between laughter-induced BOLD changes and clinical polygraphic variables. Moreover, no significant linear correlations were observed between the BOLD signal changes relative to the contrasts of interest and the number of smiles/laughter events during scanning, as well as the postscanning ratings of the videos.

Discussion

In patients with NT1, cataplexy is most often triggered by positive emotions such as those associated with laughter, joking, or delight (Overeem et al., 1999; Dauvilliers et al., 2014). For more than 130 years this striking and odd connection to positive emotions has been recognized as a key aspect of cataplexy (Schenck et al., 2007), but only recently the neural mechanisms through which positive emotions trigger cataplexy have been partly clarified in animal models (Burgess et al., 2013; Oishi et al., 2013; Dauvilliers et al., 2014).

Our study shows, for the first time in humans, the brain structures whose neural activity is specifically associated with emotion-induced cataplexy. Importantly, drug-naive children and adolescents with recent onset NT1 help us to unravel the cerebral correlates of genuine cataplexy before the patients adopt the well known behavioral strategies (“tricks”) to avoid cataplexy occurrence and before possible drug-induced effects (Overeem et al., 2011). Recent observations uncovered a discrete cataplexy phenotype in children close to disease onset. Children suffering from NT1, indeed, can present with frequent episodes of facial weakness (cataplectic facies) characterized by eyelid and mouth opening superimposed with partial or global muscle atonia triggered by emotions (Serra et al., 2008; Plazzi et al., 2011). This phenotype is more evident in younger patients close to disease onset (Plazzi et al., 2011; Pizza et al., 2013, 2014; Plazzi and Pizza, 2013) and also was reliably observed during emotional stimulation in the present fMRI paradigm.

Here, we observed that when NT1 patients showed behavioral signs of fun (smiles/laugh) without video-polygraphic evidence of cataplexy, fMRI revealed the involvement of a bilateral cortical network including the motor system and the ACC. Notably, we did not observe the involvement of cortical–subcortical regions that has been consistently observed in fMRI studies investigating humor detection and appreciation in healthy subjects, namely the mesolimbic reward areas, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, the temporo-occipital junction, the amygdala, and the periaqueductal gray (PAG; Goel and Dolan, 2001; Moran et al., 2004; Bartolo et al., 2006; Watson et al., 2007; Szameitat et al., 2010; Wattendorf et al., 2013). In contrast, when cataplexy occurred neural activity increased in a bilateral network that included cortical and subcortical regions involved in processing saliency and emotions (Mobbs et al., 2003; Seeley et al., 2007; Craig, 2009). In particular, during cataplexy BOLD signal increases encompassed the anterior insular cortex, the amygdala, the VMPFC, and the ventral striatum. Notably, we demonstrated that the cataplexy-related corticolimbic network shows an increased neural activity only when the subjects experience a high level of enjoyment. Indeed, the observed behavioral results clearly show a significant difference in the humor appreciation scores in the fMRI runs with cataplexy (i.e., higher scores) compared with the ones without cataplexy (i.e., lower scores). This behavioral difference could also explain the absence of increased BOLD signal in amygdala, insular cortex, and VMPFC during episodes of laughter without cataplexy. We can therefore infer that in NT1 cataplexy is associated with a marked increase in neural activity in suprapontine centers that physiologically process emotions and rewards. It is probable that activity in these centers, and especially in the amygdala, can in turn lead to GABAergic inhibition of pontomedullary centers, which are known to mediate REM sleep muscle atonia (Hong et al., 2006; Dauvilliers et al., 2014). Interestingly, we also disclosed cataplexy-related increased activity in pontomesencephalic regions in the proximity of the locus ceruleus and midline pontine regions that could represent the final effectors of the brain mechanism that mediates cataplexy. Importantly, it must be underscored that our results do not support any direct role of the cortical (voluntary) motor system in the suprapontine mechanisms leading to atonia in cataplexy. Indeed, when comparing cataplexy versus laughter no cortical motor–premotor area was observed to be involved.

Overall, our results are convergent with several lines of evidence supporting the crucial importance of the amygdala in cataplexy. Previous event-related functional neuroimaging studies showed abnormal amygdala activity in adult NT1 patients while seeing humorous photographic images and during aversive conditioning (Schwartz et al., 2008; Ponz et al., 2010a,b). All these studies, which investigated different aspects of emotion processing in NT1 patients, showed altered amygdala reactivity with respect to healthy volunteers. An altered response to emotional stimulation in NT1 is suggested also by neurophysiological studies disclosing a blunted autonomic, muscular cognitive response to unpleasant visual and acoustic stimuli (Tucci et al., 2003; Khatami et al., 2007). Importantly, animal models of narcolepsy strongly support an active role of amygdala neurons during cataplexy. Indeed, electrophysiological recordings from isolated cells in narcoleptic dogs demonstrate that increased activity of amygdala neurons is closely associated with cataplectic attacks (Gulyani et al., 2002), while bilateral lesions of the amygdala significantly reduce the frequency of cataplectic attacks in Hcrt−/− mice (Burgess et al., 2013).

Concerning the role of the ACC/VMPFC, several experimental studies indicate that these regions may also play a critical role in cataplexy. Prior research suggests that these areas are activated in association with positive emotions and in the affective component of humor appreciation (Goel and Dolan, 2001; Craig, 2009). Activity in the ACC, as measured by PET, is correlated with subjective feelings of happiness, and fMRI studies show that the ACC is selectively activated by pleasurable visual stimuli and reward (Damasio et al., 2000; Knutson et al., 2003; Sabatinelli et al., 2007). Importantly, in reference to narcolepsy, the ACC/VMPFC also has an established role in triggering cataplexy in animal models. The ingestion of palatable foods (for example, chocolate), which trigger cataplexy in Hcrt−/− mice, also activates neurons in the VMPFC, while inhibition of VMPFC neurons markedly reduces cataplectic attacks associated with positive emotional stimuli (Oishi et al., 2013). In addition, the ACC/VMPFC directly innervates the amygdala, which contains neurons that are active during cataplexy and innervate downstream brainstem regions involved in the regulation of muscle tone (Oishi et al., 2013). Accordingly, it has been recently reported that during cataplexy in rodent models hypersynchronous paroxysmal EEG theta bursts occur in the medial prefrontal cortex, and similar scalp paroxysmal EEG activities may appear in NT1 children close to disease onset during cataplectic events (Vassalli et al., 2013).

In this study the anterior insular cortex–frontal operculum region was the brain region with the highest increase of BOLD signal in relation to cataplexy. It is plausible that the observed insular activity represents a consequence of the autonomic and bodily changes related to cataplexy or to the visceral responses related to the pleasurable rewarding experience that immediately precedes/induces cataplexy. In this view, the anterior insular activity during cataplexy may code for the interoceptive representation of the body during humor appreciation (Moran et al., 2004; Craig, 2009). We cannot exclude that the insular cortex can exert a direct causative effect on the mechanisms leading to loss of muscle tone; however, no animal data presently support this hypothesis.

Finally, we observed a relatively asymmetric BOLD response during cataplexy that involved mainly right hemisphere corticolimbic structures. We believe that this difference can be principally attributed to the type of material used to elicit laughs (sight gags) rather than to the neurobiology of NT1. Indeed, whereas studies evaluating humor detection using material for which humor comprehension depends on language understanding showed principally left hemisphere responses (Mobbs et al., 2003), studies that used visual stimuli/sight gags uncovered more bilaterally distributed networks, or right-side prevalent BOLD responses (Watson et al., 2007).

Our findings demonstrate that the suprapontine regions whose activity is associated with cataplexy are the ones that physiologically process positive emotion and amusement/reward. These findings are in line with the well known notion that a sense of momentary muscle weakness associated with laughter can occur even in healthy individuals (Overeem et al., 1999), possibly mediated by the same neural pathway that we observed in NT1. In healthy humans, however, orexin neurons are active under conditions of high arousal and positive emotions (Mileykovskiy et al., 2005; Blouin et al., 2013), and directly innervate the ventrolateral PAG/lateral pontine tegmentum (Kaur et al., 2009; Dauvilliers et al., 2014). Therefore, in normal subjects any inhibition of these brainstem regions by the amygdala (or other suprapontine regions) would be counterbalanced by the orexin neurons and the subsequent monoamine activation, thus maintaining muscle tone (Burgess and Scammell, 2012; Clément et al., 2012). With the loss of orexin signaling in NT1 the inhibitory projections from the amygdala would go unopposed, resulting in cataplexy. Notably, we did not observe any BOLD signal changes in activity of the hypothalamic region during emotion stimulation (both during the laugh and during cataplexy) that could be the result of NT1 pathology.

Our study has limitations. First, to maximize the possibility of recording cataplexy during an fMRI session we developed a within-subject paradigm precisely tailored to the type of humor appreciated by each subject. Future studies should address whether differences are present during laughter in NT1 patients versus healthy controls beyond the occurrence of cataplexy. Indeed, our findings showed that during the laugh, in absence of cataplexy, only the voluntary motor networks for expressive laugh are present in NT1. The design of our experiment does not evaluate if the absence of “limbic” structures merely reflects the BOLD counterpart of a low level of humor detection/appreciation during the majority of laugh events or if it is related to the neurobiology of the disease. Second, BOLD imaging tells us limited information about the causal role of the different structures that were involved during cataplexy. Nevertheless, we provided for the first time a framework for the cortical and subcortical mechanisms involved in the downstream (brainstem-mediated) occurrence of cataplexy by positive emotions in humans.

Footnotes

  • We are indebted to all young patients and families participating in this study, most notably the Italian Association of Narcolepsy (AIN onlus) patients. Without their contributions, this study would not have been possible. We thank P. Baraldi and G. Pagnoni for their help and suggestions in fMRI data analysis.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to either of the following: Giuseppe Plazzi, Dipartimento di Scienze Biomediche e Neuromotorie (DIBINEM), Università di Bologna, Via Ugo Foscolo 7, 40123 Bologna, Italy, giuseppe.plazzi{at}unibo.it; or Stefano Meletti, Department of Biomedical, Metabolic, and Neural Science, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, NOCSAE Hospital, via Giardini 1355, 41126 Modena, Italy, stefano.meletti{at}unimore.it

References

Which of the following sleep disorders is strongly associated with obesity?

Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA): OSA is a sleep disorder in which the airway partially or fully collapses, causing loud snoring and breathing issues at night. OSA is seven times more common9 in individuals who are obese.

Which of the following sleep disorders is characterized by recurring problems falling or staying asleep?

Insomnia. Insomnia is characterized by the recurring difficulty to fall or remain asleep despite motivation and means to do so. People with insomnia also experience excessive daytime sleepiness and other cognitive impairments while they are awake.

What will most likely be influenced by staying up especially late on weekends?

35 Cards in this Set.