What is the name for the minimum amount of stimulation a person can detect on a given sensory channel?

Java Games: Flashcards, matching, concentration, and word search.

Tools

  • Copy this to my account
  • E-mail to a friend
  • Find other activities
  • Start over
  • Help

AB
perception The process by which people select, organize, and interpret sensory information
exposure, attention, interpretation, three stages of the process of perception
sensation The immediate response by our eyes, nose, mouth, or fingers to such basic stimuli as light, color, sound, odor, and texture
red Research has indicated that this color creates feelings of arousal and stimulates appetite
trade dress Some color combinations come to be so strongly associated with a corporation that they become known as THIS
exposure stimulus comes within the range of someone's sensory receptors
absolute THIS threshold refers to the minimum amount of stimulation that can be detected on a sensory channel
just noticeable difference The minimum difference that can be detected between two stimuli
differential THIS threshold refers to the ability of a sensory system to detect changes between two stimuli
Subliminal perception THIS occurs when a stimulus is below the level of an individual's awareness
Attention the extent to which processing activity is devoted to a particular stimulus
semiotics the filed that examines the correspondence between signs and symbols and their role in the assignment of meaning
interpretation the meanings we assign to sensory stimuli
positioning strategy THIS guides how a company uses elements of the marketing mix to influence the consumer's interpretation of the brand's meaning in the marketplace relative to its competitors.
multitasking Processing information from more than one medium at a time
Gestalt roughly means whole, pattern, or configuration or the whole is greater than the sum of its parts
symbol a sign that relates to a product by either conventional or agreed-on associations
contrast Size, color, position, and novelty are all strategies for creating THIS
sensory marketing when a gas station puts out the scent of freshly baked cinnamon rolls at the pumps to entice consumers to come inside is an example of this
sensory overload Consumers who are exposed to more information than they can process are in a state of THIS


Mrs. Gen Craft

Marketing Education/DECA

Terre Haute South High School

  • View profile
  • Send e-mail

This activity was created by a Quia Web subscriber.
Learn more about Quia
Create your own activities

Spinal Cord and Brain Stimulation

Stephen B. McMahon FMedSci, FSB, in Wall & Melzack's Textbook of Pain, 2013

Sensory Thalamic Stimulation

The first trials of stimulation in the STh nuclei were, as briefly mentioned in theIntroduction, performed byMazars and colleagues in the early 1960s (1960, 1973). Somewhat later but probably independent of the European experience, the first trials of STh stimulation were carried out in the United States (Hosobuchi et al 1973,Adams et al 1974).

Only a few experimental studies have sought to elucidate the mechanisms by which pain can be relieved by STh stimulation. In view of the similarities between this form of stimulation and SCS, it was natural to postulate the presence of supraspinal gating mechanisms. In experiments in monkeys it was shown that stimulation in the ventrobasal complex of the thalamus can inhibit spinothalamic tract WDR neurons in the DH activated both by innocuous and noxious peripheral stimuli (Gerhart et al 1983).

In an electrophysiological study designed to mimic the condition of neuropathic pain, cats were subjected to trigeminal deafferentation that resulted in increased spontaneous neuronal discharge in the spinal trigeminal nucleus (Namba and Nishimoto 1988). Stimulation, both of the sensory thalamus and the internal capsule, inhibited this deafferentation hyperactivity in almost half of the neurons, and there were also long-lasting post-stimulatory effects. A behavioral study byKupers and Gybels (1993) performed on a rat model of mononeuropathy is also of particular interest. Following partial sciatic nerve injury, these animals displayed signs of neuropathy in the form of tactile hypersensitivity in the hindpaw of the nerve-ligated leg. Stimulation applied to the sensory thalamus resulted in marked suppression of this hypersensitivity.

There is much evidence that conditions of neuropathic pain, central pain in particular, lead to profound functional changes in the sensory thalamus. A series of crucial studies using microstimulation and recording in patients during stereotactic interventions have demonstrated that in patients with such pain, the thalamic somatotopy is reorganized and there are marked signs of neural hyperexcitability and changes in response properties (e.g.,Lenz et al 1998).

Other Sensory Modalities in Sleep

Ricardo A. Velluti, in The Auditory System in Sleep (Second Edition), 2018

Summary

Sensory input and subsequent processing are definitely present in sleep, but show different characteristics than during wakefulness. The interaction between sleep and sensory physiology is an important factor because any sufficiently intense sensory stimulation always produces an awakening, from any stage of sleep.

Interestingly enough, each sensory system has an efferent pathway, with centrifugal projections ending in virtually all core afferents and on the receiver itself. Therefore, incoming sensory information can alter the physiology of sleep and wakefulness, and these states modulate incoming information.

Normal sleep depends on many aspects of sensory input. Neural networks that command sleep and wakefulness are modulated by many sensory inputs, a proportion of the ‘passive’ effects must be associated with active mechanisms of sleep. Gains or losses sensory inputs produce imbalances in neuronal networks involved in the sleep–wake cycle, changing their relative proportions of active and not being mere passive processes. For example, the almost complete deafferentation in cats caused a state of drowsiness in these animals (Vital-Durand and Michel, 1971).

Read full chapter

URL: //www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128104767000087

Physiologic and Pathophysiologic Responses to Intubation

Carin A. Hagberg MD, FASA, in Hagberg and Benumof's Airway Management, 2018

Technical Considerations: Minimizing Stimulation of Airway Sensory Receptors

As a general rule, cardiovascular responses to airway maneuvers can be minimized by limiting airway sensory receptor stimulation, starting with manipulation of the larynx itself. In a double-blind study, cricoid pressure with 4.5 kg of force resulted in a significantly greater HR and BP response to endotracheal intubation than occurred in patients whose cricoid area was gently palpated.29 This underrecognized effect of cricoid pressure should be considered when estimating the risk-benefit ratio of this procedure in individual patients.

Laryngoscopy is a moderately stimulating procedure, and the use of a straight blade (e.g., a Miller blade) with elevation of the vagally innervated posterior aspect of the epiglottis results in significantly higher arterial BP than does the use of a curved blade (e.g., a Macintosh blade).30 Video-assisted laryngoscopy (VAL), which does not require alignment of the anatomical axes for adequate visualization of the glottis and subsequent intubation, has the potential to minimize the pressor response to airway manipulation. This is evidenced by manikin studies demonstrating that VAL requires less force to displace oropharyngeal tissues than direct laryngoscopy (DL) with a Macintosh blade.31,32

Use of the Pentax-AWS video laryngoscope (Pentax, Tokyo, Japan) has been reported to attenuate the hemodynamic response to endotracheal intubation after fentanyl/propofol induction when compared with either the GlideScope (Verathon, Bothell, WA) or the Macintosh laryngoscope (Fig. 7.2).33 This finding is not universal. An earlier study comparing the Pentax-AWS to Macintosh laryngoscopy reported no significant differences in systolic BP, diastolic BP, or HR after intubation, and two separate studies comparing the GlideScope and Macintosh laryngoscope also failed to find significant differences in the hemodynamic response to intubation with these devices.34,35 One possible explanation for the lack of hemodynamic advantage to less stimulating intubation devices is that the act of endotracheal intubation is far more hemodynamically stimulating than laryngoscopy itself. For example, the use of a lighted intubation stylet fails to prevent hemodynamic stimulation once an ETT is advanced past the vocal cords.36

Insertion of a laryngeal mask airway (LMA) after induction of general anesthesia with thiopental or propofol and fentanyl has been shown to cause a lower cardiovascular and endocrine response than laryngoscopy or endotracheal intubation.37–40 The LMA has the advantage of avoiding the vagally-mediated infraglottic stimulation entailed by the use of a laryngoscope, thus requiring lighter levels of general anesthesia. Furthermore, because neuromuscular blockade is not required for airway control, spontaneous ventilation and avoidance of the adverse hemodynamic consequences of PPV are possible. In contrast, endotracheal intubation via an intubating LMA results in a hemodynamic and endocrine response similar to that of DL and intubation after propofol induction.41 Therefore if endotracheal intubation is necessary, there may not be a hemodynamic advantage to instrumenting the airway with the intubating LMA or other less stimulating devices. Notably, placement of a Combitube (Kendall-Sheridan Catheter Corp., Argyle, NY) was found to cause significantly greater elevations in BP and catecholamine release when compared with endotracheal intubation or LMA placement.40

Disorders of Consciousness: Coma, Vegetative State, and Minimally Conscious State

Susan Grieve, in Physical Rehabilitation, 2007

Sensory Stimulation.

Sensory stimulation is the application of environmental stimuli by an external agent for the purpose of promoting arousal and behavioral responsiveness.84 Formalized sensory stimulation programs as a treatment for patients in a coma or vegetative state became popular in the 1980s despite a lack of scientific evidence proving or disproving their effectiveness. The programs varied in intensity and frequency of intervention, as well as targeted senses. At a minimum, most programs included stimulation of visual, auditory, olfactory, kinesthetic, and tactile senses.

Proponents of such programs contended that patients with disorders of consciousness suffered from environmental deprivation and that this “deprivation could lead to widespread impairments of intellectual and perceptual processes accompanied by changes in cerebral electrical activity.”85 Controversy concerning the benefits of such programs continued throughout the 1990s, prompting the Cochrane Collaboration in 1999 to formally assess the effectiveness of sensory stimulation programs through a systematic review of the literature. Including only randomized controlled trials that compared sensory stimulation with standard rehabilitation programs, the Cochrane group found “no reliable evidence to support or rule out the effectiveness of multi-sensory programs in patients in coma or vegetative state.”86 There is no literature on the effectiveness of sensory stimulation programs in patients in minimally conscious state. Clinicians need to understand the high degree of uncertainty of outcomes with these programs and take into account other prognostic factors for outcome before initiating or recommending sensory stimulation intervention.

Read full chapter

URL: //www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780721603612500247

DETECTION AND GENERATION OF ELECTRIC SIGNALS | Active Electrolocation

G. von der Emde, J. Engelmann, in Encyclopedia of Fish Physiology, 2011

Information Processing in ELL

ELL circuitry

Sensory input of mormyromast afferents is relayed to a layer of granule cells in the ELL. Here, the temporal code of the afferents is transformed into a rate code. Granule cells are either gamma-aminobuytric acid-ergic (GABAergic) or glutamatergic and make synapses on the basal dendrites of both inhibitory interneurons and two types of efferent neurons. Efferent neurons that are inhibited by sensory input are the large ganglion (LG) cells, while large fusiform (LF) cells are excited by sensory input. The most prominent interneurons, the Purkinje-like medium ganglion (MG) cells, provide inhibitory input to these efferent cells (Figure 8).

Figure 8. (a) Photomicrograph of a section through the ELL of Gnathonemus. Laminar regions of the medial zone of the ELL are highlighted in the same colors as used in the schematic of the ELL (b). (b) Schematic of the layered organization of the ELL. On the left and below, sources of input to the ELL are shown. Primary sensory input from electroreceptors converges on granular cells in the granular layer. These cells also receive strong corollary discharge-related input from the juxtalobar nucleus (electric organ corollary discharge (EOCD)). Local interneurons (black) and ELL efferent cells of two varieties (blue and red) are found in the ganglionic layer (output layer). While large ganglionic cells (I-cells, blue) are inhibited by sensory input, large fusiform cells (E-cells, red) are excited. The local interneurons (medium ganglionic neurons, black) are either inhibited or excited by the sensory input and provide local inhibitory feedback to the efferent cells. Afferents of nucleus preeminentialis supply direct electrosensory feedback input to the basal apical dendrites of these cells of the ganglionic layer. A third central source of afferents comes from the eminentia granularis (EgP), which sends massive bundles of parallel fibers to the molecular layer of the ELL. This input conveys corollary discharge input (EOCD), sensory input of other modalities, as well as predictive signals that drive plasticity in this system.

The photo was kindly provided by K. Grant.

In addition to the peripheral sensory input, several central afferents provide projections to the ELL (Figure 8). These central afferents are related to specific neuronal tasks such as noise cancellation, the differentiation of re- and exafferent input, and the decoding of the latency code of the sensory afferents (see also DETECTION AND GENERATION OF ELECTRIC SIGNALS | Physiology of Tuberous Electrosensory Systems).

Read full chapter

URL: //www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123745538001349

Behavioral Embryology

In Studies on the Development of Behavior and the Nervous System, 1973

Sensory stimulation can have a number of different influences during embryonic development, among which are the following. (1) It can affect the ongoing overt motility of the embryo, either exciting movement or inhibiting it. (2) Particular patterns of sensory stimulation encountered at an early age—independent of their immediate effect on motor movement—may influence the perception and behavior of the embryo or neonate at a later age. Of course, these two effects of sensory stimulation are not mutually exclusive, but it is important to realize that (1) can occur without (2) and vice versa. So these are rather different propositions which require different experimental techniques for their verification or analysis—each effect must be documented in its own right, and that is what we shall see in the ensuing articles by Margaret Vince and by Monica Impekoven and Peter Gold.

Read full chapter

URL: //www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780126093018500167

Neuroplasticity

Sergio Bagnato, in Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 2022

Sensory stimulation

Sensory stimuli, experiences, and emotions continuously influence brain structure. The rationale for using sensory stimulation with the aim of promoting plasticity and recovering consciousness in patients with DOCs comes from protocols of environmental enrichment that, as previously described, are associated with positive biologic and behavioral effects in experimental conditions. The assumptions of this approach lie in the concept that environmental changes after a severe brain injury manifest as virtual isolation of the patient (for example, during an intensive care unit stay) and have potentially detrimental effects on his/her recovery. However, there are significant differences between the environmental enrichment adopted in animal models and sensory stimulation, essentially depending on the fact that the first creates the conditions for a high level of behavioral interactions, whereas the second requires that specific stimuli be actively administered. There is no consensus on what stimuli should be used, but it is generally accepted that stimuli with emotional salience and autobiographic content may be more effective in promoting consciousness recovery (Abbate et al., 2014). Studies with different techniques suggest that seeing the patient's own face or familiar faces (Sharon et al., 2013; Bagnato et al., 2015) or listening to the patient's own name (Kempny et al., 2018) may lead to more effective activation of specific brain areas rather than stimuli without autobiographic content, especially in patients who are in an MCS. Similarly, a patient's preferred flavor or smell is generally used for stimulation rather than generic flavors or smells. A recent study showed that sensory stimulation based mainly on autobiographic content (a picture of the patient's closest family member; his/her favorite music, flavor and smell; and nonspecific tactile stimulation of the arms) may lead to some improvements in responsiveness in patients with MCS but not in patients with UWS (Cheng et al., 2018). Because the awareness of one's own experiences and personality (self-awareness) and the awareness of the external world (perceptual awareness) involve different cortical areas (Tacikowski et al., 2017), sensory stimulation programs should include stimuli with both autobiographic and non-autobiographic content to maximize the chances of achieving cortical activation and plastic changes. Despite the encouraging theoretical premises, the current evidence for the use of sensory stimulation in patients with DOCs is still low, and no standardized procedures are widely accepted. The increasing diffusion of virtual reality systems for cognitive rehabilitation will likely facilitate the adoption of new and more standardized programs of sensory stimulation in patients with DOCs over the coming years.

Read full chapter

URL: //www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128194102000205

Sensory System Changes

Donna J. Cech DHS, PT, PCS, Suzanne “Tink” Martin MACT, PT, in Functional Movement Development Across the Life Span (Third Edition), 2012

Summary

Sensory input plays an important role in the learning and refinement of movement. The integration of multiple sensory input and the association-coordination of sensory and motor information form the basis for cognition and perception. Sensory input from the external environment initiates the activity that shapes synaptic connections. The autonomic nervous system and the reticular system act as gatekeepers, determining which sensory information reaches consciousness and which is dampened. The thalamus is a central relay station that directs the flow of sensory information to association cortices. All sensory systems have similar characteristics: transduction and coding, representation, and integration at all levels of the nervous system. Sensory systems develop early in utero to be ready to function at birth. Some, such as vision and hearing, must have additional input to completely develop the neural pathway. Sensory abilities change with age; deficits in any sensory system, whether from congenital absence, trauma, or decline with age, can result in functional impairment of movement. Presbystasis, presbyopia, and presbycusis are all seen in older adults but not to the same degree in all individuals. Age-related changes in the sensory systems, therefore, are neither uniform nor universal.

Read full chapter

URL: //www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9781416049784000107

Preconscious Awareness

Edgar Garcia-Rill PhD, in Waking and the Reticular Activating System in Health and Disease, 2015

Abstract

Sensory input induces gamma band activity in the reticular activating system that participates in preconscious awareness, the process necessary to support a state capable of reliably assessing the world around us on a continuous basis. This process is manifested rapidly upon waking, following increased blood flow in the thalamus and brain stem that occurs ahead of increases in cortical blood flow. This mechanism is also involved in the preconscious activity necessary for the preparation for voluntary movements. This occurs in advance of the subjective sensation of will or intention to move, that is, preconsciously.

Read full chapter

URL: //www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128013854000100

Repetitive Stress Pathology

Nancy N. Byl, ... Grant Glass, in Pathology and Intervention in Musculoskeletal Rehabilitation (Second Edition), 2016

Sensory Training (Braille)

Sensory stimulation can functionally reorganize the brain.259 This occurs not only after injury267 but with heavy hand use like in musicians250,251 and braille readers.525 However, cortical de-differentiation is reported in patients with FHd294 and animals trained intensively until the development of involuntary, uncontrolled hand movements when performing the target task.75,296,299,300 Improved cortical differentiation is reported in multifinger braille readers.525 Thus, teaching patients to read braille523,524 and perform other sensory discrimination activities522,535,536 has been recommended as a method of driving the reorganization needed to restore motor control in patients with FHd.

Read full chapter

URL: //www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780323310727000270

Which of the following represents the minimum amount of stimulation a person can detect on a given sensory channel?

The absolute threshold is the minimum amount of stimulation required for a person to detect the stimulus 50 percent of the time.

What is the smallest amount of stimulation needed for detection?

The smallest amount of stimulation needed for detection is known as the absolute threshold.

When a person can detect stimulation this is called?

The just noticeable difference (JND), also known as the difference threshold, is the minimum level of stimulation that a person can detect 50% of the time.

What is it called when sensory stimulation occurs below a person's absolute threshold?

Answer and Explanation: In concrete terms, the sensory stimulation that occurs below a person's absolute threshold for conscious perception is called subliminal perception.

Toplist

Neuester Beitrag

Stichworte